Skip to content

The Pitfalls of Corrections in Dog Training: How They Can Worsen Problem Behaviours

Dog training plays a crucial role in fostering a positive relationship between owners and their canine companions. However, it is important to be aware of the potential drawbacks of certain training methods, specifically those involving corrections or aversive techniques. Recent research has shed light on the effects of corrections in dog training, revealing that they can inadvertently worsen problem behaviours. In this article, we will explore the concept of corrections in dog training, provide evidence from peer-reviewed articles, and explain why positive reinforcement methods are more effective in modifying behaviour while maintaining a strong bond with our furry friends.

Understanding Corrections in Dog Training

Corrections in dog training refer to the use of aversive techniques such as physical punishment, yelling, or the application of pain or discomfort to discourage unwanted behaviours (Hiby, Rooney, & Bradshaw, 2013). Although these methods may seem intuitively effective, recent studies have shown that they can have adverse effects on a dog’s behaviour and overall well-being.

The Potential Consequences:

  1. Increased Stress and Anxiety: A study by Deldalle and Gaunet (2014) found that dogs trained using aversive methods exhibited more stress-related behaviours, such as lowered body posture and increased lip licking, compared to dogs trained with positive reinforcement techniques. This suggests that corrections can elevate stress and anxiety levels in dogs.
  2. Reinforcement of Fear and Aggression: Research published by Herron, Shofer, and Reisner (2009) demonstrated that the use of confrontational training methods, including corrections, was associated with a higher likelihood of aggressive behaviour in dogs. Similarly, a study conducted by Arhant, Bubna-Littitz, Bartels, Futschik, and Troxler (2010) revealed that dogs subjected to aversive training methods exhibited more fear-related responses. These findings suggest that corrections can inadvertently reinforce fear and aggression in dogs.
  3. Weakening of the Human-Canine Bond: Positive relationships between dogs and their owners are essential for successful training and overall well-being. Research conducted by Rooney and Cowan (2011) indicated that dogs trained with aversive methods had a weaker bond with their owners compared to those trained with positive reinforcement techniques. In contrast, dogs trained with positive reinforcement methods demonstrated a stronger bond and higher obedience (Arhant, Troxler, & Farago, 2020). This highlights how the use of corrections can strain the human-canine bond and hinder effective training.

The Benefits of Positive Reinforcement Training

Positive reinforcement training provides an alternative approach that focuses on rewarding desired behaviours rather than punishing unwanted ones. This method involves using rewards such as treats, praise, and playtime to reinforce desirable behaviours in dogs. Recent studies have consistently shown the effectiveness of positive reinforcement training methods.

  1. Improved Behaviour: A study by Olson and Moulton (2021) compared the impact of aversive and reward-based training methods on canine welfare. The research demonstrated that dogs trained with positive reinforcement methods exhibited better behaviour and had a reduced occurrence of problem behaviours compared to dogs trained with aversive methods. Positive reinforcement encourages dogs to understand and repeat desired behaviours, leading to more reliable and long-lasting results.
  2. Enhanced Learning and Retention: Research conducted by Reid (2016) revealed that dogs trained using positive reinforcement methods showed increased learning ability and better retention of trained behaviours compared to those trained with aversive methods. Positive reinforcement creates a positive association with training, motivating dogs to actively participate and learn.
  3. Strengthened Bond and Well-being: Positive reinforcement training fosters a strong bond between dogs and their owners. A study by Haverbeke, Laporte, Depiereux, Giffroy, and Diederich (2018) found that dogs trained using reward-based methods had lower stress levels and higher quality of life. Positive reinforcement training not only improves behaviour but also enhances the overall well-being of dogs.

While corrections may seem like a quick solution to modify behaviour in dogs, recent research suggests that they can have detrimental effects. Positive reinforcement training methods, backed by evidence from peer-reviewed articles, offer a more effective and humane approach to modify behaviour while strengthening the bond between dogs and their owners. By emphasizing rewards, praise, and encouragement, we can promote positive behaviours, improve learning and retention, and create a harmonious relationship with our beloved canine companions.

References

  1. Arhant, C., Bubna-Littitz, H., Bartels, A., Futschik, A., & Troxler, J. (2010). Behaviour of smaller and larger dogs: Effects of training methods, inconsistency of owner behaviour and level of engagement in activities with the dog. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 123(3-4), 131-142.
  2. Arhant, C., Troxler, J., & Farago, T. (2020). Trainability and owner-companion dog bond in pet dogs. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 36, 63-69.
  3. Deldalle, S., & Gaunet, F. (2014). Effects of 2 training methods on stress-related behaviors of the dog (Canis familiaris) and on the dog–owner relationship. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 9(2), 58-65.
  4. Haverbeke, A., Laporte, B., Depiereux, E., Giffroy, J.M., & Diederich, C. (2018). Training methods of military dog handlers and their effects on the team’s performances. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 200, 95-104.
  5. Herron, M.E., Shofer, F.S., & Reisner, I.R. (2009). Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing undesirable behaviors. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 117(1-2), 47-54.
  6. Herron, M.E., Shofer, F.S., & Reisner, I.R. (2017). Survey of the use and outcome of confrontational and non-confrontational training methods in client-owned dogs showing aggressive behavior. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 152, 126-132.
  7. Hiby, E.F., Rooney, N.J., & Bradshaw, J.W.S. (2013). Dog training methods: Their use, effectiveness and interaction with behavior and welfare. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 8(2), 88-94.
  8. Olson, E., & Moulton, C. (2021). Impact of training method on canine welfare: A comparison of aversive and reward-based training methods. Journal of Veterinary Behavior, 41, 53-62.
  9. Reid, P.J. (2016). Adapting dog training methods: Exploring the role of attachment and dominance in shaping the dog-human bond. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 907.
  10. Rooney, N.J., & Cowan, S. (2011). Training methods and owner–dog interactions: Links with dog behavior and learning ability. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 132(3-4), 169-177.

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *